Noticing

From emcawiki
Revision as of 21:39, 17 October 2023 by ChaseRaymond (talk | contribs) (Created page with "{{Infobox cite | Authors = '''Danielle Pillet-Shore''' (University of New Hampshire, USA) (https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4757-4082) | To cite = Pillet-Shore, Danielle. (2023)....")
(diff) ← Older revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)
Jump to: navigation, search
Encyclopedia of Terminology for CA and IL: Noticing
Author(s): Danielle Pillet-Shore (University of New Hampshire, USA) (https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4757-4082)
To cite: Pillet-Shore, Danielle. (2023). Noticing. In Alexandra Gubina, Elliott M. Hoey & Chase Wesley Raymond (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Terminology for Conversation Analysis and Interactional Linguistics. International Society for Conversation Analysis (ISCA). DOI:


The term noticing is used to describe:

(i) a private, individual, perceptual/cognitive action of bringing one’s attention or conscious awareness to a target phenomenon/experience (e.g., a yoga class instruction to ‘notice your breath,’ or becoming aware of a strong smell as one enters a new space; cf. Robinson, 1995; Schegloff, 2007);

(ii) a public, social action of calling joint attention to a target referent (e.g., Goodwin & Goodwin, 2012:274; Kääntä, 2014; Pillet-Shore, 2021; Sacks, 1992; Schegloff, 1988; 2007). <\blockquote>

Conversation analytic work examines noticing as a social/interactional event, which “need not be engendered by a perceptual/cognitive one. And many (perhaps most) perceptual/cognitive noticings do not get articulated interactionally at all. But one key normative trajectory is an interactional noticing presented as occasioned by a perceptual/cognitive one” (Schegloff, 2007:87). As an action-in-conversation, noticing involves the selection and presentation of a target referent—some feature of the setting and/or its participants that is publicly and concurrently perceivable (Pillet-Shore, 2021; Schegloff, 2007:219) and thus available for mutual scrutiny (Goodwin & Goodwin, 2012). Though many studies examine the action of noticing visible referents (e.g., while driving in a car, Goodwin & Goodwin, 2012; in a classroom, Kääntä, 2014), others also examine the action of noticing (or otherwise commenting upon) referents that are audible, olfactible, palpable (Pillet-Shore, 2021) and/or tasteable (e.g., Mondada, 2012; Vannini, Ahluwalia-Lopez, Waskul & Gottschalk, 2010).

Participants to interaction use various multimodal (spoken and/or embodied; visible and/or audible) resources to do the action of noticing (Goodwin & Goodwin, 2012; Kääntä, 2014; Pillet-Shore, 2021; Sacks, 1992:81-94; Schegloff, 2007:87). These include utterances that audibly point (Pillet-Shore, 2021:11-12) to a selected target referent (e.g., using deictic terms [Hindmarsh & Heath, 2000], perceptual directives [Goodwin & Goodwin, 2012:275-279], and/or response cries [Goffman, 1978]). These also include other audible and visible actions such as smiling and laughter, sudden gaze shifts, intensified gaze to the target referent accompanied by a facial expression that displays stance, and sharing a mutual glance with a companion (Kääntä, 2014; Pillet-Shore, 2021; Schegloff, 2007:87-88, footnote 17).

The action of noticing mobilizes joint sensorial attention (Goodwin & Goodwin, 2012:274; Kidwell & Zimmerman, 2007; Pillet-Shore, 2021) to/on “features which it formulates or registers, but treats them as its source, while projecting the relevance of some further action in response” (Schegloff, 2007:219). Noticing thus constitutes a first pair part (FPP) in an adjacency pair sequence that makes “relevant a sharing of the noticing” (Schegloff, 2007:74-75), initiating a “retro-sequence” (ibid:217-19). There is debate, however, about whether noticing embodies conditional relevance, with one view suggesting that depends upon how it is visibly and audibly designed (Stivers & Rossano, 2010:9, 17-18). Noticing is a practice often used to introduce a new topic and initiate a new sequence of interaction (e.g., during conversation openings [Pillet-Shore, 2008; 2021] and at a places of possible sequence completion, including after a lapse in conversation [Hoey, 2018]). Noticings are accountable events that participants treat as interactionally consequential (Kääntä, 2014; Kidwell, 2009; Pillet-Shore, 2021).

Studies investigating this social action use various terms to refer to it, including not only “noticing” (e.g., Goodwin & Goodwin, 2012; Kääntä, 2014; Sacks, 1992; Schegloff, 1988; 2007) but also “announcing” (Schegloff, 2007:86-87; Stivers & Rossano, 2010:9), “registering” (e.g., Hoey, 2018; Pillet-Shore, 2008; 2017; 2018; 2021; Schegloff 2007:87), “setting talk” (Maynard & Zimmerman, 1984:304), “comments on the physical surroundings” (Keevallik, 2018), and “local sensitivity to elements in participants’ field of perception” (Bergmann, 1990:207). Among these, Sacks (1992 II:87–97) and Schegloff (1988:119–131) use “noticing” as a vernacular term to invoke and parlay “the reader’s experience” without offering a definition (Schegloff, 2007:88).

Scholars’ usage of these terms—particularly “noticing” and “announcing”—is inconsistent. For instance, when analyzing cases of car talk, while Schegloff (2007:74) uses both of the terms “noticing” and “announcing” to describe the utterance “There’s MahCo:”, Goodwin & Goodwin (2012:276-277) use only the term “noticing” to describe a similar utterance, “There’s the fire up there guys.” Such inconsistency leaves unclear what makes an action recognizable (to participants, and then to analysts) as doing “noticing” versus “announcing” a publicly and concurrently perceivable referent. Consider the bolded utterances in Excerpts (1) and (2):

(1) [PT25]

01 Tea: =hh[h! ((T gazing at table/moving student work)) 02 Dad: [hheh huh 03 Mom: hhih hu[h ((M gazing toward table)) 04 Dad: -> [Oh wo:w.=Look at that.= 05 Tea: =.hhuh! h[ih hih 06 Mom: [hhh hh* ((*D extends his right arm, 07 pointing his index finger at a guillotine paper trimmer 08 on the floor)) 09 Dad: ^Is tha:t a sa:fe way ((^T’s/M’s gaze follow D’s point)) 10 [to leave that big bla:d*e¿ ((*D lowers his arm)) 11 Mom: [hhih! Hah [hah hoh hoh 12 Tea: [No:::, 13 Tea: You’re noticing all our: [(.) Yih know what? This is= 14 M?: [.hh ( ) 15 Tea: =a room that’s been not used¿ 16 Mom: Fer a while,= ((T, D bending to pick up paper trimmer)) 17 Tea: =Yeah.=


(2) [Holidayc-1 (Pillet-Shore, 2021:27-28)]

01 ((dog Bailey puts front paws up on D’s side)) 02 Dav: Awo:h,=Good heavens Bay leaf¿= ((D lifts Bailey)) 03 Kik: =nhhm! 04 Cla: hhhh! 05 Dav: [Come (on/here)= ((D hugging Bailey)) 06 Ale: [Bay leaf,= 07 Cla: =O:h B(h)ay lea[f? 08 Kik: -> [Look.=He’s dre:ssed* up. 09 Cla: [Aw:, ((*K moves rt hand to touch dog’s collar)) 10 Kik: [Did you notice his outfit?= 11 Dav: =Uh- Di:d I. Why do you think I’m holding him. According to one view (Goodwin & Goodwin, 2012:268, 276), both Excerpt (1)-line 4 and Excerpt (2)-line 8 contain “resources for constructing noticings.” These resources include: Excerpt (1)’s response cry or exclamatory interjection (“Oh wo:w”; Goffman, 1978), perceptual directive (“Look”), and deictic term (“that”); and Excerpt (2)’s perceptual directive (“Look”) and categorization/description (“He’s dre:ssed up”). From this perspective, both bolded utterances work to “bring a seeable field into view” and summon “someone to look at a particular phenomenon,” and thus are describable as doing “noticing.” According to a second view (Schegloff, 2007:82), the bolded utterances in Excerpts (1) and (2) differ: whereas Excerpt (1)-line 4 shows a “noticing-by-other,” Excerpt (2)-line 8 shows an “announcement-by-self” (where ‘self’ is the one characterized by the feature)”—the participant regarded as most responsible for the target referent (Pillet-Shore, 2021). From this perspective, the referent in Excerpt (1)—a guillotine paper trimmer laying on the floor with its blade in an open position—has just now become perceivable to the speaker Dad (note his turn-initial reaction token [Heritage, 1984:286–7]), who is not regarded by other participants as in any way responsible for it. Since Dad is a guest entering a school meeting room for a conference with his child’s kindergarten teacher (who is acting as host and regarded as more responsible for this referent), Dad is doing a “noticing-by-other.” In Excerpt (2), however, the referent—a colorful collar wrapped around the neck of a pet dog—has been perceivable to the speaker Kiki for some time, since Kiki is standing in her own home welcoming guests to her party, for which she has specifically “dressed up” her own dog. Thus, since Kiki is regarded by other participants as responsible for her dog’s appearance, she is doing an “announcement-by-self.”

A third view (Pillet-Shore, 2021:13-16) empirically demonstrates that it can be difficult to analytically disaggregate “noticing” from “announcing” a here-and-now referent. To solve this terminological problem, this view proposes the term “registering” to name the basic underlying social action for what it is apparently designed to do—to multimodally register or call joint attention to a selected publicly and concurrently perceivable referent so others shift their sensory attention to it. Excerpts (1) and (2) both show participants registering by using linguistic and embodied resources to call joint attention to a target referent (in Excerpt (1)-lines 4, 6-8; and Excerpt (2)-lines 8-9), with fellow participants responding by observably shifting their displayed attention to that target referent (see Pillet-Shore [2021] for analysis of registering sequences, including how participants produce and understand their actions guided by a structural preference organization).


Additional Related Entries:


Cited References:

Brown, P. & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Clayman, S. E. (2002). Sequence and solidarity. In Advances in group processes (pp. 229-253). Emerald Group Publishing Limited, Bingley.

Clayman, S. E., & Raymond, C. W. (2021). You know as invoking alignment: A generic resource for emerging problems of understanding and affiliation. Journal of Pragmatics, 182, 293–309.

Davidson, J. (1984). Subsequent versions of invitations, offers, requests, and proposals dealing with potential or actual rejection. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (eds), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis (pp. 102–28). Cambridge, UK. Cambridge University Press.

Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays in face to face behavior. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.

Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and ethnomethodology. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.

Heritage, J. (2015). Well-prefaced turns in English conversation: A conversation analytic perspective. Journal of Pragmatics, 88, 88-104.

Kendrick, K. & Drew, P. (2014). The putative preference for offers over requests. In P. Drew & E. Couper-Kuhlen (eds.), Requesting in social interaction (pp. 87-113). John Benjamins.

Kendrick, K. & Torreira, F. (2015). The timing and construction of preference: A quantitative study. Discourse Processes, 52(4), 255-289.

Lerner, G. H. (1996). Finding “face” in the preference structures of talk-in-interaction. Social Psychology Quarterly, 59(4), 303-321.

Pillet-Shore, D. (2010). Making way and making sense: Including newcomers in interaction. Social Psychology Quarterly, 73(2), 152-175.

Pillet-Shore, D. (2011). Doing introductions: The work involved in meeting someone new. Communication Monographs, 78(1), 73-95.

Pillet-Shore, D. (2012a). Greeting: Displaying stance through prosodic recipient design. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 45(4), 375-398.

Pillet-Shore, D. (2012b). The problems with praise in parent-teacher interaction. Communication Monographs, 79(2), 181-204.

Pillet-Shore, D. (2015). Being a “good parent” in parent-teacher conferences. Journal of Communication, 65(2), 373-395.

Pillet-Shore, D. (2016). Criticizing another’s child: How teachers evaluate students during parent-teacher conferences. Language in Society, 45(1), 33-58.

Pillet-Shore, D. (2017). Preference organization. In Oxford research encyclopedia of communication, edited by J. Nussbaum. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Pillet-Shore, D. (2021). When to make the sensory social: Registering in face-to-face openings. Symbolic Interaction, 44(1), 10-39.

Pillet-Shore, D. (in press, 2023). Where the action is: Positioning matters in interaction. Chapter 22 in J. Robinson, R. Clift, K. Kendrick & C. W. Raymond (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of methods in conversation analysis. Cambridge University Press.

Pomerantz, A. (1984). Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shapes. In J. M. Atkinson & J. C. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action (pp. 57–101). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Pomerantz, A. & Heritage, J. (2013). Preference. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 210-228). Oxford: John Wiley and Sons.

Robinson, J. D. (2020). Revisiting preference organization in context: A qualitative and quantitative examination of responses to information seeking. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 53(2), 197-222.

Robinson, J. D. & Bolden, G. (2010). Preference organization of sequence-initiating actions: The case of explicit account solicitations. Discourse Studies, 12(4): 501-533.

Sacks, H. (1987). On the preferences for agreement and contiguity in sequences in conversation. In G. Button & J. R. E. Lee (Eds.), Talk and social organization (pp. 54-69). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

Schegloff, E. A. (2007). Sequence organization in interaction: A Primer in conversation analysis (Vol. 1). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Schegloff, E. A., & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening up closings. Semiotica, 8, 289–327.

Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G. & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language, 53(2), 361-82.

Sidnell, J. (2010). Conversation analysis: An introduction. Wiley-Blackwell.

Steensig, J. (2020). Conversation analysis and affiliation and alignment. In C. A. Chapelle (ed.), The concise encyclopedia of applied linguistics (pp. 248-253). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.

Stivers, T. (2008). Stance, alignment and affiliation during storytelling: When nodding is a token of affiliation. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 41(1), 31-57.

Stivers, T. & Robinson, J. (2006). A preference for progressivity in interaction. Language in Society, 35, 367-392.


Additional References:


EMCA Wiki Bibliography items tagged with 'dispreferred'