Difference between revisions of "Abrupt-join"

From emcawiki
Jump to: navigation, search
 
Line 1: Line 1:
 
{{Infobox cite
 
{{Infobox cite
 
| Authors = '''Marit Aldrup''' (University of Potsdam) (https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7208-6822)
 
| Authors = '''Marit Aldrup''' (University of Potsdam) (https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7208-6822)
| To cite =  Aldrup, Marit. (2023). Abrupt-join. In Alexandra Gubina, Elliott M. Hoey & Chase Wesley Raymond (Eds.), ''Encyclopedia of Terminology for Conversation Analysis and Interactional Linguistics''. International Society for Conversation Analysis (ISCA). DOI: []
+
| To cite =  Aldrup, Marit. (2023). Abrupt-join. In Alexandra Gubina, Elliott M. Hoey & Chase Wesley Raymond (Eds.), ''Encyclopedia of Terminology for Conversation Analysis and Interactional Linguistics''. International Society for Conversation Analysis (ISCA). DOI: [https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/VRQGW 10.17605/OSF.IO/VRQGW]
 
}}
 
}}
  

Latest revision as of 19:48, 21 December 2023

Encyclopedia of Terminology for CA and IL: Abrupt-join
Author(s): Marit Aldrup (University of Potsdam) (https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7208-6822)
To cite: Aldrup, Marit. (2023). Abrupt-join. In Alexandra Gubina, Elliott M. Hoey & Chase Wesley Raymond (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Terminology for Conversation Analysis and Interactional Linguistics. International Society for Conversation Analysis (ISCA). DOI: 10.17605/OSF.IO/VRQGW


Abrupt-joins are tempo-related turn-holding devices which allow current speakers to extend their turn beyond a possible completion point. Like rush-throughs, they are employed to locally preempt turn transition by means of temporally compressing an upcoming transition-relevance place (TRP) and launching another turn-constructional unit (TCU) before a co-participant can self-select for next speakership (Sacks, et al. 1974). Both practices thus facilitate the ad hoc construction of multi-unit turns.

In contrast to rush-throughs, which have been described as integrative in terms of their prosodic-phonetic design, abrupt-joins constitute a disjunctive turn-holding practice: Aside from the close temporal and articulatory proximity between first and second TCU, the second TCU features discontinuities of pitch, loudness as well as tempo and typically also changes the projected trajectory of talk (Local & Walker 2004; Couper-Kuhlen & Selting 2018: 90f.).

The following example taken from Local and Walker (2004: 1381) is a case in point. The TCU "what do you think of Ann Percy" (line 7) is latched right onto the prior one at a possible completion point (see juncture marked by “►”) with a compressed last syllable, but designed disjunctively with a pitch upstep, a slightly slower articulation rate and an increase in loudness. By quickly launching a new topic at a point of imminent topic closure, Robbie secures both her right to the floor and the opportunity to unilaterally effect a sudden shift in the direction of talk:

[Holt.5.88.1.5.20.finger] (Local & Walker 2004: 1381)

01   Rob:    I just fee::l:- (0.4) if they’re going to go the
02           wa::y: of the modern schoo:ls there’s an awf:ul-
03           they’re caught between the two that’s their pro[blem
04   Les:                                                   [that’s ri:ght
05           (0.3)
06   Rob:    and they’ve got to go:: (.) you know really get
07        -> their finger out► what do you think of Ann Percy
08           (.)
09   Les:    .hhhhh we::ll do you kno:w e-I wuh- I: have a
10           certain sneaking respect for her

Overall, abrupt-joins are characterized by the following set of prosodic-phonetic features (Local & Walker 2004):

  • Localized acceleration with subsequent deceleration: The final syllable of the first TCU is produced with a noticeably faster articulation rate. At the same time, there is often a marked slowing down at the beginning of the second TCU.
  • Pitch: The first TCU has turn-final pitch characteristics and there is a noticeable pitch upstep at the beginning of the second TCU.
  • Loudness: The first stressed syllable of the second TCU is noticeably louder than the last syllable of the first TCU.
  • Articulatory encroachment: Certain phonetic parameters, such as place or manner of articulation, may be assimilated across the join of the first and the second TCU. When the first TCU ends and the second TCU begins with a sonorant, voicing is typically maintained across the join.


Additional Related Entries:


Cited References:

Couper-Kuhlen, E. & Selting, M. (2018). Interactional Linguistics. Studying Language in Social Interaction. Cambridge University Press.

Local, J. K. & Walker, G. (2004). Abrupt-joins as a resource for the production of multi-unit, multi-action turns. Journal of Pragmatics, 36(8), 1375–1403.

Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A. & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735.


Additional References:

Clayman, S. E. (2013). Turn-constructional units and the transition-relevance place. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.) The Handbook of Conversation Analysis, (pp. 151–166). Wiley-Blackwell.


EMCA Wiki Bibliography items tagged with 'abrupt-join'