Difference between revisions of "Groeber-Pochon-Berger2014"
PaultenHave (talk | contribs) (Created page with "{{BibEntry |BibType=ARTICLE |Author(s)=Simone Groeber; Evelyne Pochon-Berger; |Title=Turns and turn-taking in sign language interaction: A study of turn-final holds |Tag(s)=EM...") |
AndreiKorbut (talk | contribs) m |
||
Line 3: | Line 3: | ||
|Author(s)=Simone Groeber; Evelyne Pochon-Berger; | |Author(s)=Simone Groeber; Evelyne Pochon-Berger; | ||
|Title=Turns and turn-taking in sign language interaction: A study of turn-final holds | |Title=Turns and turn-taking in sign language interaction: A study of turn-final holds | ||
− | |Tag(s)=EMCA; Sign Language; Turn-taking; | + | |Tag(s)=EMCA; Sign Language; Turn-taking; |
|Key=Groeber-Pochon-Berger2014 | |Key=Groeber-Pochon-Berger2014 | ||
|Year=2014 | |Year=2014 | ||
|Journal=Journal of Pragmatics | |Journal=Journal of Pragmatics | ||
|Volume=65 | |Volume=65 | ||
− | |Pages= | + | |Pages=121–136 |
+ | |URL=http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0378216613002026 | ||
+ | |DOI=10.1016/j.pragma.2013.08.012 | ||
+ | |Abstract=This article examines a recurrent phenomenon in sign language interaction: the freezing of a sign, called a ‘hold’, in turn-final position. This phenomenon is traditionally described as a prosodic feature that contributes to the rhythm of signed talk and to the marking of syntactic boundaries, hence not adding any propositional content on its own. A detailed observation of these holds in naturally occurring conversational data, however, raises the following questions: What is the relevance of such holds in the management of turn-taking? What meaningful social action do they accomplish? Based on 90 min of video-recordings of Swiss German Sign Language (DSGS) interaction within an institutional setting, we undertake micro-sequential and multimodal analyses yielding the following findings (1) turn-final holds occur recurrently in turns that set a strong action projection (e.g. questions), (2) they embody the current speaker's expectations regarding next actions; and therefore (3) their release is finely tuned to the recognizability of the relevant and expected next action in progress. | ||
}} | }} |
Revision as of 11:22, 11 March 2016
Groeber-Pochon-Berger2014 | |
---|---|
BibType | ARTICLE |
Key | Groeber-Pochon-Berger2014 |
Author(s) | Simone Groeber, Evelyne Pochon-Berger |
Title | Turns and turn-taking in sign language interaction: A study of turn-final holds |
Editor(s) | |
Tag(s) | EMCA, Sign Language, Turn-taking |
Publisher | |
Year | 2014 |
Language | |
City | |
Month | |
Journal | Journal of Pragmatics |
Volume | 65 |
Number | |
Pages | 121–136 |
URL | Link |
DOI | 10.1016/j.pragma.2013.08.012 |
ISBN | |
Organization | |
Institution | |
School | |
Type | |
Edition | |
Series | |
Howpublished | |
Book title | |
Chapter |
Abstract
This article examines a recurrent phenomenon in sign language interaction: the freezing of a sign, called a ‘hold’, in turn-final position. This phenomenon is traditionally described as a prosodic feature that contributes to the rhythm of signed talk and to the marking of syntactic boundaries, hence not adding any propositional content on its own. A detailed observation of these holds in naturally occurring conversational data, however, raises the following questions: What is the relevance of such holds in the management of turn-taking? What meaningful social action do they accomplish? Based on 90 min of video-recordings of Swiss German Sign Language (DSGS) interaction within an institutional setting, we undertake micro-sequential and multimodal analyses yielding the following findings (1) turn-final holds occur recurrently in turns that set a strong action projection (e.g. questions), (2) they embody the current speaker's expectations regarding next actions; and therefore (3) their release is finely tuned to the recognizability of the relevant and expected next action in progress.
Notes