Difference between revisions of "Walker2018"

From emcawiki
Jump to: navigation, search
(Created page with "{{BibEntry |BibType=ARTICLE |Author(s)=Gareth Walker |Title=Close proximity of turn-continuation to possible turn-completion in conversation |Tag(s)=EMCA; Conversation; Phonet...")
 
 
Line 9: Line 9:
 
|Journal=Speech Communication
 
|Journal=Speech Communication
 
|Volume=99
 
|Volume=99
|Pages=231-241
+
|Pages=231–241
|DOI=https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2018.02.006
+
|URL=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S016763931630173X
|Abstract=Speakers in conversation are in the first instance allocated the space in which to produce a single unit of talk which could figure as a complete turn. One way speakers can continue talk beyond a point of possible turn-completion is by producing a continuation in maximally close proximity to the point of possible turn-completion.
+
|DOI=10.1016/j.specom.2018.02.006
This article extends our understanding of how speakers do this, and to what interactional effect. Several phonetic features characterising close-proximity continuations are described. These include continuation of voicing, articulatory anticipation and other phonetic features. It is argued that close-proximity continuation helps ensure
+
|Abstract=Speakers in conversation are in the first instance allocated the space in which to produce a single unit of talk which could figure as a complete turn. One way speakers can continue talk beyond a point of possible turn-completion is by producing a continuation in maximally close proximity to the point of possible turn-completion. This article extends our understanding of how speakers do this, and to what interactional effect. Several phonetic features characterising close-proximity continuations are described. These include continuation of voicing, articulatory anticipation and other phonetic features. It is argued that close-proximity continuation helps ensure speakers get to continue their turn past a point of possible turn-completion. This argument is supported by close inspection of the talk leading up to the point of possible turn-completion, the start of the continuation, and the responses of co-participants – especially with regard to whether the point of possible turn-completion engenders incoming talk or not. The method combines analysis of phonetic and sequential details of 240 points of possible turn-completion in audio recordings of unscripted American English telephone calls.
speakers get to continue their turn past a point of possible turn-completion. This argument is supported by close inspection of the talk leading up to the point of possible turn-completion, the start of the continuation, and the responses of co-participants – especially with regard to whether the point of possible turn-completion engenders
 
incoming talk or not. The method combines analysis of phonetic and sequential details of 240 points of possible turn-completion in audio recordings of unscripted American English telephone calls.
 
 
}}
 
}}

Latest revision as of 04:48, 11 January 2020

Walker2018
BibType ARTICLE
Key Walker2018
Author(s) Gareth Walker
Title Close proximity of turn-continuation to possible turn-completion in conversation
Editor(s)
Tag(s) EMCA, Conversation, Phonetics, Pragmatics, Prosody, Turn-continuation, Turn-taking
Publisher
Year 2018
Language English
City
Month
Journal Speech Communication
Volume 99
Number
Pages 231–241
URL Link
DOI 10.1016/j.specom.2018.02.006
ISBN
Organization
Institution
School
Type
Edition
Series
Howpublished
Book title
Chapter

Download BibTex

Abstract

Speakers in conversation are in the first instance allocated the space in which to produce a single unit of talk which could figure as a complete turn. One way speakers can continue talk beyond a point of possible turn-completion is by producing a continuation in maximally close proximity to the point of possible turn-completion. This article extends our understanding of how speakers do this, and to what interactional effect. Several phonetic features characterising close-proximity continuations are described. These include continuation of voicing, articulatory anticipation and other phonetic features. It is argued that close-proximity continuation helps ensure speakers get to continue their turn past a point of possible turn-completion. This argument is supported by close inspection of the talk leading up to the point of possible turn-completion, the start of the continuation, and the responses of co-participants – especially with regard to whether the point of possible turn-completion engenders incoming talk or not. The method combines analysis of phonetic and sequential details of 240 points of possible turn-completion in audio recordings of unscripted American English telephone calls.

Notes