Difference between revisions of "Kristiansen-etal2017"
PaultenHave (talk | contribs) (Created page with "{{BibEntry |BibType=ARTICLE |Author(s)=Elisabeth Dalby Kristiansen; Ann Katrine Marstrand; Jalal El Derbas; |Title=Repeating a Searched-For Word With an Agreement Token in “...") |
AndreiKorbut (talk | contribs) |
||
Line 3: | Line 3: | ||
|Author(s)=Elisabeth Dalby Kristiansen; Ann Katrine Marstrand; Jalal El Derbas; | |Author(s)=Elisabeth Dalby Kristiansen; Ann Katrine Marstrand; Jalal El Derbas; | ||
|Title=Repeating a Searched-For Word With an Agreement Token in “Challenged Interaction” | |Title=Repeating a Searched-For Word With an Agreement Token in “Challenged Interaction” | ||
− | |Tag(s)=EMCA; Word searches; Epistemic authority; Second Language; Dementia; Membership; Communicative competence; | + | |Tag(s)=EMCA; Word searches; Epistemic authority; Second Language; Dementia; Membership; Communicative competence; |
|Key=Kristiansen-etal2017 | |Key=Kristiansen-etal2017 | ||
|Year=2017 | |Year=2017 | ||
Line 11: | Line 11: | ||
|Number=4 | |Number=4 | ||
|Pages=388-403 | |Pages=388-403 | ||
+ | |URL=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/08351813.2017.1375803 | ||
|DOI=10.1080/08351813.2017.1375803 | |DOI=10.1080/08351813.2017.1375803 | ||
− | |Abstract=When one is searching for a word, another person may provide it. This | + | |Abstract=When one is searching for a word, another person may provide it. This article is about what happens next—specifically, what happens when the first speaker repeats the offered word and adds an agreement token, before or after. We analyze this practice across three data sets of “challenged interaction” (second language interaction in Danish, and English and Danish atypical interaction). When a “challenged” search initiator (e.g., a person with dementia) puts the agreement token after the repeat, that claims epistemic authority and demonstrates competence; conversely, if they put the agreement token before the repeat, that seems to defer to others’ claims of epistemic authority and competence. The article contributes to conversation analytic studies of atypical interaction and deviance by describing how speakers in “challenged interaction” deal with competence as a practical problem. Data is in English, Arabic, and Danish with English translations. |
− | article is about what happens next—specifically, what happens when the | ||
− | first speaker repeats the offered word and adds an agreement token, before | ||
− | or after. We analyze this practice across three data sets of “challenged | ||
− | interaction” (second language interaction in Danish, and English and | ||
− | Danish atypical interaction). When a “challenged” search initiator (e.g., a | ||
− | person with dementia) puts the agreement token after the repeat, that | ||
− | claims epistemic authority and demonstrates competence; conversely, if | ||
− | they put the agreement token before the repeat, that seems to defer to | ||
− | others’ claims of epistemic authority and competence. The article | ||
− | |||
− | by describing how speakers in “challenged interaction” deal with | ||
− | |||
− | English translations. | ||
}} | }} |
Latest revision as of 06:26, 13 September 2023
Kristiansen-etal2017 | |
---|---|
BibType | ARTICLE |
Key | Kristiansen-etal2017 |
Author(s) | Elisabeth Dalby Kristiansen, Ann Katrine Marstrand, Jalal El Derbas |
Title | Repeating a Searched-For Word With an Agreement Token in “Challenged Interaction” |
Editor(s) | |
Tag(s) | EMCA, Word searches, Epistemic authority, Second Language, Dementia, Membership, Communicative competence |
Publisher | |
Year | 2017 |
Language | English |
City | |
Month | |
Journal | Research on Language and Social Interaction |
Volume | 50 |
Number | 4 |
Pages | 388-403 |
URL | Link |
DOI | 10.1080/08351813.2017.1375803 |
ISBN | |
Organization | |
Institution | |
School | |
Type | |
Edition | |
Series | |
Howpublished | |
Book title | |
Chapter |
Abstract
When one is searching for a word, another person may provide it. This article is about what happens next—specifically, what happens when the first speaker repeats the offered word and adds an agreement token, before or after. We analyze this practice across three data sets of “challenged interaction” (second language interaction in Danish, and English and Danish atypical interaction). When a “challenged” search initiator (e.g., a person with dementia) puts the agreement token after the repeat, that claims epistemic authority and demonstrates competence; conversely, if they put the agreement token before the repeat, that seems to defer to others’ claims of epistemic authority and competence. The article contributes to conversation analytic studies of atypical interaction and deviance by describing how speakers in “challenged interaction” deal with competence as a practical problem. Data is in English, Arabic, and Danish with English translations.
Notes