Difference between revisions of "Morita2015"
SaulAlbert (talk | contribs) (Ok, I figured it out - it seems square brackets in titles are also a problem to sort out in version 2 of the wiki...) |
PaultenHave (talk | contribs) m |
||
Line 7: | Line 7: | ||
|Year=2014 | |Year=2014 | ||
|Journal=Discourse Processes | |Journal=Discourse Processes | ||
− | |Number= | + | |Volume=52 |
− | |Pages= | + | |Number=4 |
+ | |Pages=290-310 | ||
|URL=http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774 | |URL=http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774 | ||
|DOI=10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774 | |DOI=10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774 | ||
|Abstract=This study investigates a particular behavior in talk-in-interaction that appears to be, at least in its most explicit form, relatively unique to children, that is, the behavior whereby one participant explicitly instructs another participant to say a specific phrase, after which the first participant then supplies a prefashioned response. Rather than simply dismissing such conduct as the product of an immature psychology, close examination of the sequential organization of such talk reveals that even children at a very early age comprehend the execution of any given speech action demands a particular sequential context for that talk to be both sense-making and effective. The data discussed herein reveal an understanding of preference organization and sequential trajectory displayed by children as young as 2 and 3 years old and that this three-part "say [x]" format has versatile application in children's talk-in-interaction. | |Abstract=This study investigates a particular behavior in talk-in-interaction that appears to be, at least in its most explicit form, relatively unique to children, that is, the behavior whereby one participant explicitly instructs another participant to say a specific phrase, after which the first participant then supplies a prefashioned response. Rather than simply dismissing such conduct as the product of an immature psychology, close examination of the sequential organization of such talk reveals that even children at a very early age comprehend the execution of any given speech action demands a particular sequential context for that talk to be both sense-making and effective. The data discussed herein reveal an understanding of preference organization and sequential trajectory displayed by children as young as 2 and 3 years old and that this three-part "say [x]" format has versatile application in children's talk-in-interaction. | ||
}} | }} |
Revision as of 04:03, 10 July 2015
Morita2015 | |
---|---|
BibType | ARTICLE |
Key | Morita2014 |
Author(s) | Emi Morita |
Title | “Say (x)”: A Device for Securing Conversational Footing in the Talk of Young Children |
Editor(s) | |
Tag(s) | Children, Sense-making, Footing, EMCA, In press |
Publisher | |
Year | 2014 |
Language | |
City | |
Month | |
Journal | Discourse Processes |
Volume | 52 |
Number | 4 |
Pages | 290-310 |
URL | Link |
DOI | 10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774 |
ISBN | |
Organization | |
Institution | |
School | |
Type | |
Edition | |
Series | |
Howpublished | |
Book title | |
Chapter |
Abstract
This study investigates a particular behavior in talk-in-interaction that appears to be, at least in its most explicit form, relatively unique to children, that is, the behavior whereby one participant explicitly instructs another participant to say a specific phrase, after which the first participant then supplies a prefashioned response. Rather than simply dismissing such conduct as the product of an immature psychology, close examination of the sequential organization of such talk reveals that even children at a very early age comprehend the execution of any given speech action demands a particular sequential context for that talk to be both sense-making and effective. The data discussed herein reveal an understanding of preference organization and sequential trajectory displayed by children as young as 2 and 3 years old and that this three-part "say [x]" format has versatile application in children's talk-in-interaction.
Notes