Difference between revisions of "Morita2015"

From emcawiki
Jump to: navigation, search
m
 
(2 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 2: Line 2:
 
|BibType=ARTICLE
 
|BibType=ARTICLE
 
|Author(s)=Emi Morita;
 
|Author(s)=Emi Morita;
|Title=“Say (x)”: A Device for Securing Conversational Footing in the Talk of Young Children
+
|Title=“Say (x)”: a device for securing conversational footing in the talk of young children
|Tag(s)=Children; Sense-making; Footing; EMCA; In press
+
|Tag(s)=Children; Sense-making; Footing; EMCA;
|Key=Morita2014
+
|Key=Morita2015
|Year=2014
+
|Year=2015
 
|Journal=Discourse Processes
 
|Journal=Discourse Processes
 
|Volume=52
 
|Volume=52
 
|Number=4
 
|Number=4
|Pages=290-310
+
|Pages=290–310
|URL=http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774
+
|URL=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774
 
|DOI=10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774
 
|DOI=10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774
 
|Abstract=This study investigates a particular behavior in talk-in-interaction that appears to be, at least in its most explicit form, relatively unique to children, that is, the behavior whereby one participant explicitly instructs another participant to say a specific phrase, after which the first participant then supplies a prefashioned response. Rather than simply dismissing such conduct as the product of an immature psychology, close examination of the sequential organization of such talk reveals that even children at a very early age comprehend the execution of any given speech action demands a particular sequential context for that talk to be both sense-making and effective. The data discussed herein reveal an understanding of preference organization and sequential trajectory displayed by children as young as 2 and 3 years old and that this three-part "say [x]" format has versatile application in children's talk-in-interaction.
 
|Abstract=This study investigates a particular behavior in talk-in-interaction that appears to be, at least in its most explicit form, relatively unique to children, that is, the behavior whereby one participant explicitly instructs another participant to say a specific phrase, after which the first participant then supplies a prefashioned response. Rather than simply dismissing such conduct as the product of an immature psychology, close examination of the sequential organization of such talk reveals that even children at a very early age comprehend the execution of any given speech action demands a particular sequential context for that talk to be both sense-making and effective. The data discussed herein reveal an understanding of preference organization and sequential trajectory displayed by children as young as 2 and 3 years old and that this three-part "say [x]" format has versatile application in children's talk-in-interaction.
 
}}
 
}}

Latest revision as of 06:08, 15 December 2019

Morita2015
BibType ARTICLE
Key Morita2015
Author(s) Emi Morita
Title “Say (x)”: a device for securing conversational footing in the talk of young children
Editor(s)
Tag(s) Children, Sense-making, Footing, EMCA
Publisher
Year 2015
Language
City
Month
Journal Discourse Processes
Volume 52
Number 4
Pages 290–310
URL Link
DOI 10.1080/0163853X.2014.955774
ISBN
Organization
Institution
School
Type
Edition
Series
Howpublished
Book title
Chapter

Download BibTex

Abstract

This study investigates a particular behavior in talk-in-interaction that appears to be, at least in its most explicit form, relatively unique to children, that is, the behavior whereby one participant explicitly instructs another participant to say a specific phrase, after which the first participant then supplies a prefashioned response. Rather than simply dismissing such conduct as the product of an immature psychology, close examination of the sequential organization of such talk reveals that even children at a very early age comprehend the execution of any given speech action demands a particular sequential context for that talk to be both sense-making and effective. The data discussed herein reveal an understanding of preference organization and sequential trajectory displayed by children as young as 2 and 3 years old and that this three-part "say [x]" format has versatile application in children's talk-in-interaction.

Notes