Difference between revisions of "Lindegaard2014"

From emcawiki
Jump to: navigation, search
 
Line 3: Line 3:
 
|Author(s)=Laura Bang Lindegaard
 
|Author(s)=Laura Bang Lindegaard
 
|Title=Doing focus group research: Studying rational ordering in focus group interaction
 
|Title=Doing focus group research: Studying rational ordering in focus group interaction
|Tag(s)=EMCA; Focus Groups; MCA                              
+
|Tag(s)=EMCA; Focus Groups; MCA
 
|Key=Lindegaard2014
 
|Key=Lindegaard2014
 
|Year=2014
 
|Year=2014
Line 9: Line 9:
 
|Volume=16
 
|Volume=16
 
|Number=5
 
|Number=5
|Pages=629-644
+
|Pages=629–644
|URL=http://dis.sagepub.com/content/16/5/629.abstract
+
|URL=https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1461445614538563
 
|DOI=10.1177/1461445614538563
 
|DOI=10.1177/1461445614538563
 
|Abstract=Scholars of ethnomethodologically informed discourse studies are often sceptical of the use of interview data such as focus group data. Some scholars quite simply reject interview data with reference to a general preference for so-called naturally occurring data. Other scholars acknowledge that interview data can be of some use if the distinction between natural and contrived data is given up and replaced with a distinction between interview data as topic or as resource. In greater detail, such scholars argue that interview data are perfectly adequate if the researcher wants to study the topic of interview interaction, but inadequate as data for studying phenomena that go beyond the phenomenon of interview interaction. Neither of these more and less sceptical positions are, on the face of it, surprising due to the ethnomethodological commitment to study social order as accomplished in situ, not as something that pre-exists or goes beyond the situated interaction. This article, however, challenges not only the first, but also the second position and suggests that it is, after all, possible to do committedly ethnomethodological studies of focus group data that demonstrate how members of a focus group setting accomplish certain rational orders, and, significantly, how they do so by utilizing certain available resources.
 
|Abstract=Scholars of ethnomethodologically informed discourse studies are often sceptical of the use of interview data such as focus group data. Some scholars quite simply reject interview data with reference to a general preference for so-called naturally occurring data. Other scholars acknowledge that interview data can be of some use if the distinction between natural and contrived data is given up and replaced with a distinction between interview data as topic or as resource. In greater detail, such scholars argue that interview data are perfectly adequate if the researcher wants to study the topic of interview interaction, but inadequate as data for studying phenomena that go beyond the phenomenon of interview interaction. Neither of these more and less sceptical positions are, on the face of it, surprising due to the ethnomethodological commitment to study social order as accomplished in situ, not as something that pre-exists or goes beyond the situated interaction. This article, however, challenges not only the first, but also the second position and suggests that it is, after all, possible to do committedly ethnomethodological studies of focus group data that demonstrate how members of a focus group setting accomplish certain rational orders, and, significantly, how they do so by utilizing certain available resources.
 
}}
 
}}

Latest revision as of 09:36, 9 December 2019

Lindegaard2014
BibType ARTICLE
Key Lindegaard2014
Author(s) Laura Bang Lindegaard
Title Doing focus group research: Studying rational ordering in focus group interaction
Editor(s)
Tag(s) EMCA, Focus Groups, MCA
Publisher
Year 2014
Language
City
Month
Journal Discourse Studies
Volume 16
Number 5
Pages 629–644
URL Link
DOI 10.1177/1461445614538563
ISBN
Organization
Institution
School
Type
Edition
Series
Howpublished
Book title
Chapter

Download BibTex

Abstract

Scholars of ethnomethodologically informed discourse studies are often sceptical of the use of interview data such as focus group data. Some scholars quite simply reject interview data with reference to a general preference for so-called naturally occurring data. Other scholars acknowledge that interview data can be of some use if the distinction between natural and contrived data is given up and replaced with a distinction between interview data as topic or as resource. In greater detail, such scholars argue that interview data are perfectly adequate if the researcher wants to study the topic of interview interaction, but inadequate as data for studying phenomena that go beyond the phenomenon of interview interaction. Neither of these more and less sceptical positions are, on the face of it, surprising due to the ethnomethodological commitment to study social order as accomplished in situ, not as something that pre-exists or goes beyond the situated interaction. This article, however, challenges not only the first, but also the second position and suggests that it is, after all, possible to do committedly ethnomethodological studies of focus group data that demonstrate how members of a focus group setting accomplish certain rational orders, and, significantly, how they do so by utilizing certain available resources.

Notes