Difference between revisions of "Levinson2015"

From emcawiki
Jump to: navigation, search
m
Line 3: Line 3:
 
|Author(s)=Stephen C. Levinson; Francisco Torreira;
 
|Author(s)=Stephen C. Levinson; Francisco Torreira;
 
|Title=Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language
 
|Title=Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language
|Tag(s)=EMCA; Turn-taking; , conversation; language processing; language production; language comprehension; timing; quantification; Psychology; psycholinguistics
+
|Tag(s)=EMCA; Turn-taking; language processing; language production; language comprehension; timing; quantification; Psychology; psycholinguistics; conversation
 
|Key=Levinson2015
 
|Key=Levinson2015
 
|Year=2015
 
|Year=2015

Revision as of 03:48, 7 May 2019

Levinson2015
BibType ARTICLE
Key Levinson2015
Author(s) Stephen C. Levinson, Francisco Torreira
Title Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language
Editor(s)
Tag(s) EMCA, Turn-taking, language processing, language production, language comprehension, timing, quantification, Psychology, psycholinguistics, conversation
Publisher
Year 2015
Language
City
Month
Journal Frontiers in Psychology
Volume 6
Number 731
Pages
URL Link
DOI 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00731
ISBN
Organization
Institution
School
Type
Edition
Series
Howpublished
Book title
Chapter

Download BibTex

Abstract

The core niche for language use is in verbal interaction, involving the rapid exchange of turns at talking. This paper reviews the extensive literature about this system, adding new statistical analyses of behavioural data where they have been missing, demonstrating that turn-taking has the systematic properties originally noted by Sacks, Schegloff and Jefferson (1974; hereafter SSJ). This system poses some significant puzzles for current theories of language processing: the gaps between turns are short (of the order of 200 ms), but the latencies involved in language production are much longer (over 600 ms). This seems to imply that participants in conversation must predict (or 'project' as SSJ have it) the end of the current speaker's turn in order to prepare their response in advance. This in turn implies some overlap between production and comprehension despite their use of common processing resources. Collecting together what is known behaviourally and experimentally about the system, the space for systematic explanations of language processing for conversation can be significantly narrowed, and we sketch some first model of the mental processes involved for the participant preparing to speak next.

Notes